Introduction
In the treatment of mental health or behavior problems of children and adolescents, involving the family can be an important part of the intervention. Given the influence of family functioning on child and adolescent development (Rutter
2002), treatment to target problematic family functioning and to enhance protective family factors can be vital in reducing youth psychopathology. Indeed, results of several randomized controlled trials support the effectiveness of family-based treatment models for youth problems, such as attachment-based family therapy (ABFT; Diamond et al.
2010), multidimensional family therapy (MDFT; Henderson et al.
2010; Rigter et al.
2013), functional family therapy (FFT; Hartnett et al.
2016; Sexton and Turner
2011) and family-based therapy (FBT; Couturier et al.
2013; Lock et al.
2010). Moreover, in comparative meta-analytic reviews on the effectiveness of treatment for youth delinquency (Latimer
2001), adolescent substance abuse (Tanner-Smith et al.
2013) and anorexia nervosa (Lock et al.
2010), family treatment models have been shown to be more effective than interventions for youth only.
Over the past years, delivery of family-based interventions for youth has become more integrative and flexible, and interventions that combine individual therapy, family treatment and sometimes medication have become increasingly popular (Diamond and Josephson
2005). An example of such an integrative intervention is family-based cognitive behavioral therapy (FB CBT), which has shown to be efficacious for treatment of pediatric obsessive–compulsive disorder (O’Leary et al.
2009; Storch et al.
2007) and anxiety disorders (Ginsburg and Schlossberg
2002; Kendall et al.
2008).
In order to gain better understanding of the effectiveness of family-involved interventions, it is important to know what components or conditions of treatment cause positive outcomes. Previous research has shown that the alliance between therapists and clients is a significant predictor of treatment outcome in individual youth psychotherapy as well as family therapy (Friedlander et al.
2011; McLeod
2011; Shirk et al.
2011).
Most research on alliance is based on Bordin’s (
1979) definition of the alliance which he developed for the individual therapy context, also referred to as therapeutic or working alliance. Bordin argues that the professional relationship between a therapist and client consists of three components: (a) an emotional bond between therapist and client based on mutual trust and sympathy, (b) agreement on which problems and goals are the central issue in therapy and (c) agreement on tasks that need to be performed by therapist and client in order to achieve central goals.
The process of building and maintaining an emotional bond and agreement on tasks and goals raises several complexities in working with families. In family-involved treatment, the therapist simultaneously develops multiple alliances with family members who are in treatment together, but who differ in their characters, needs and treatment expectations (Kindsvatter and Lara
2012; Rait
2000). For instance, in a study on alliance and treatment outcome in home-based family therapy by Johnson et al. (
2002) the correlation between alliance and outcome was stronger for fathers than for mothers. For fathers, the agreement with the therapist about treatment goals was more predictive of treatment outcome than the agreement on tasks and the emotional bond, whereas for mothers agreement on tasks was relatively more predictive of treatment effectiveness. In addition, research showed that treatment effectiveness can be reduced when the therapist develops a stronger alliance with one family member than with the other: These unbalanced or so-called split alliances increase the risk of treatment drop out (Flicker et al.
2008; Robbins et al.
2003).
Another complicating aspect of building and maintaining alliances in family-involved treatment is that each person’s alliance with the therapist is observed and influenced by the other participating family members (Friedlander et al.
2006; Kindsvatter and Lara
2012). These observations might cause feelings of unsafety or anxiety, since what is said during a session can have repercussions outside therapy sessions. For example, a teenage son who tells the therapist about a relapse in drug abuse with his parents present might be worried about getting punished at home for this relapse. Thus, the therapist needs to provide guidelines or discuss basic rules of safety and confidentiality in order to gain confidence and trust from all participating family members (Friedlander et al.
2006).
A third aspect of alliance specific to family-involved treatment is that treatment outcome is not only affected by multiple individual alliances between therapist and family members, but also by the alliance with family as a whole (Escudero et al.
2008; Friedlander et al.
2008; Kindsvatter and Lara
2012). When family members perceive themselves as a group collaborating to improve family functioning and achieve other therapeutic goals, treatment is more likely to be effective. Therefore, family therapists must leverage different views on problems and solutions within the family and try to bring about a shared sense of common family goals by for example emphasizing shared values and experiences (Escudero et al.
2008; Friedlander et al.
2006; Rait
2000).
Perhaps because of these complexities in alliance processes specific to family-involved treatment, research on alliance in this field emerged later and received far less attention than research on alliance in individual psychotherapy. In the 1980s, Pinsof and Catherall (
1986) applied Bordin’s definition of alliance to three interpersonal levels by measuring bonds, tasks and goals for three relationships: self with therapist, other with therapist and group with therapist. This approach was elaborated on by Pinsof (
1994) when he added the
within-
family alliance, namely the extent to which family members collaborate on goals and tasks and experience an emotional bond with each other during therapy. Symonds and Horvath (
2004) defined this concept as
allegiance. Friedlander et al. (
2006) elaborated on Bordin’s definition of alliance as well as family therapy-specific alliance processes, such as allegiance, by distinguishing four domains of alliance in family therapy: (a) emotional connection to the therapist, (b) engagement in the therapy, (c) shared sense of purpose within the family (similar to Pinsof’s
within-
family alliance) and (d) safety within the therapeutic system. The two latter domains are said to be unique to conjoint family therapy.
To date, only one meta-analytic review on the association between alliance and outcome in family-involved treatment has been published (Friedlander et al.
2011). This study investigated the alliance–outcome correlation in 16 family therapy studies and 8 couple therapy studies. The result of the analysis was an average weighted effect size of
r = .24 for the family therapy studies, demonstrating that higher levels of alliance are associated with more positive treatment outcome. This overall effect size is comparable to the effect size in meta-analyses on alliance and outcome in individual adult and youth psychotherapy (Horvath et al.
2011; Shirk et al.
2011).
Although Friedlander et al.’s (
2011) meta-analysis provides a valuable test of the association between alliance and outcome in family therapy, the study also underlines the importance of further meta-analytical research on alliance in family-involved treatment for two reasons. First, the study included only 16 family therapy studies published until 2008. Since then, scientific attention for alliance processes in family-involved treatment research has burgeoned, resulting in an increase in studies on the subject. Second, the study reported significant variability in the correlation between alliance and outcome. This is not surprising, because the studies that were included in the meta-analysis showed a large heterogeneity with regard to alliance measures and other methodological aspects. This variety within and between studies was dealt with by collapsing several alliance measures (e.g., multiple types of alliance, informants, measurement instruments and measurement moments) into one effect size per study. No distinction was made between different types of alliance processes, and no moderator analyses were conducted. Therefore, the reported variability between studies remained unexplained.
Different Types of Alliance Processes in Family-Involved Treatment
In research on the association between alliance and outcome in family-involved treatment, different types of alliance processes can be distinguished. A first type of alliance is the more traditional fixed moment measure of the level of alliance. Alliance can be measured at the start, middle or end of therapy, or at multiple moments, emphasizing that alliance is an ongoing process rather than a fixed state concept (Horvath
2006; Karver and Carporino
2010). In addition, some studies use alliance change scores to investigate whether the improvement of alliance during the therapy process influences treatment outcome (e.g., Bachler et al.
2016; Keeley et al.
2011). The relevance of this second type of alliance is illustrated by a study on alliance in adolescent psychotherapy, demonstrating that alliance change scores explain more variance in treatment outcome compared to single-moment measures or an average of multiple single-moment measures (Owen et al.
2016).
A third type of alliance refers to so-called split or unbalanced alliances and addresses the systemic aspect of alliance in family-involved treatment. Multiple family members form alliances with the therapist, which might differ in strength. When one family member has a better alliance with the therapist than other family members (i.e., alliances with the therapist are unbalanced between family members), this is generally referred to as a “split alliance.” Some studies have investigated whether these split alliances affect treatment outcome by subtracting family members’ single alliance scores and correlating these discrepancy scores with treatment outcome. When discrepancy scores are investigated, a negative correlation with treatment outcome is expected (i.e., higher levels of unbalance lead to less favorable treatment outcomes) instead of a positive correlation, as is the general hypothesis in research on the level of individual or family alliance and outcome.
Moderators of the Alliance: Outcome Association
The association between alliance and outcome can be moderated by several factors. Several methodological aspects of studies might have a moderating effect, as has been reported in meta-analyses on alliance and outcome in youth and adult psychotherapy (Horvath et al.
2011; McLeod
2011; Shirk and Karver
2003; Shirk et al.
2011). First, it is important to investigate whether study quality moderates the alliance–outcome association: When higher quality studies indicate a stronger effect, this might be an indication of the robustness of the association. Second, timing of alliance measurement can be an important moderator. Alliance might be a predictor of outcome early in treatment, underlining the importance of alliance as a facilitator of successful therapy. On the other hand, meta-analyses in youth psychotherapy (McLeod
2011; Shirk and Karver
2003) and adult psychotherapy (Horvath et al.
2011) have indicated that alliance might be more predictive of outcome when assessed in a later stage of treatment, as it may need some time to build.
It might furthermore be of influence whose perspective on alliance as well as on outcome is measured (parent, youth, therapist or observer). Especially in family-involved treatment, with multiple family members involved, it is important to know what perspective is most predictive of successful treatment. Meta-analyses on alliance in youth psychotherapy either suggest that the parents’ or the therapists’ perspective on the alliance is most predictive of outcome (McLeod
2011; Shirk and Karver
2003) and that children’s reports on the alliance show very little variability (Shirk and Karver
2003). In addition, alliance seems to be most predictive of therapeutic outcome as perceived by either the parent (McLeod
2011) or the therapist (Shirk and Karver
2003) when compared to youth or observer reported outcome.
A methodological feature specific to studies on alliance in family-involved treatment is whether the alliance is measured at an individual (e.g., parent–therapist, youth–therapist) or family level (the alliance between the therapist and the family as a whole) using instruments specifically designed for family interventions. These instruments not only investigate individual alliances between family members and therapist, but additionally address the within-group or group-with-therapist aspects of alliance typical of family interventions. The moderating effect of type of alliance in family therapy is illustrated in a study by Escudero et al. (
2008), in which the within-family alliance was correlated more strongly with outcome than the individual alliances. However, not all studies on alliance in family-involved treatment use instruments designed to measure family aspects of the alliance as well as individual alliances. As pointed out by McLeod (
2011), the correlation between alliance and outcome in family-involved treatment might be stronger when the alliance measure is designed to investigate alliance processes typical of working with multiple family members.
Aside from methodological features of studies, several treatment aspects could moderate the effect of alliance on outcome. First, treatment models differ in the extent to which alliance building aspects of treatment are specified. Some treatment models explicitly describe alliance building stages of treatment (ABFT, Feder and Diamond
2016) or therapeutic practices to build multiple alliances (FFT, Sexton and Alexander,
2004; MDFT, Liddle
2002). For other treatment models, such as family-based CBT (Freeman et al.
2003), no specific alliance building stages or techniques are described. For the latter, the correlation between alliance and outcome might be smaller than for treatment models with a strong emphasis on alliance building practices.
Also, referral to treatment was shown to have a moderating effect in a meta-analytic review on alliance in youth psychotherapy in a way that correlations between alliance and outcome were found to be stronger for help-seeking youth than for youth receiving mandated treatment (McLeod
2011). Another moderating treatment aspect might be the setting in which treatment is conducted. When treatment is (partially) home-based, the therapist enters the home environment of the family. Effectiveness of the treatment might therefore be more dependent on the degree to which the family feels at ease with and trusts the therapist.
Furthermore, sample characteristics can moderate the association between alliance and outcome. In three meta-analytic reviews on alliance in youth psychotherapy, it has been shown that the nature of patients’ problems was a moderating factor: In two reviews, alliance correlated more strongly with outcome for youth with externalizing problem behavior than for youth with internalizing problems (McLeod
2011; Shirk and Karver
2003). A third review indicated that for youth dealing with substance abuse and mixed problems alliance correlated more strongly with treatment outcome than for youth dealing with eating disorders (Shirk et al.
2011). In two of these meta-analytic reviews, age of youth also proved to have a moderating effect, with stronger correlations between alliance and outcome for younger children compared to adolescents (McLeod
2011; Shirk and Karver
2003). Another moderating sample characteristic is shown in a study on alliance and outcome in home-based family therapy, where a stronger correlation between alliance and outcome was found for fathers than for mothers (Johnson et al.
2002). This suggests that gender can moderate the effect of alliance on outcome.
Lastly, it can be reasoned that cultural differences play a role in how important the alliance is in enhancing favorable treatment outcomes, especially in family-involved treatment. For example, in more collectivist cultures the within-family alliance or the extent to which alliances with multiple family members are unbalanced might be of more influence on treatment outcome compared to more individualist cultures. This is illustrated in a study on ethnic background, therapeutic alliance and retention in functional family therapy (FFT), in which unbalanced alliances between family members predicted treatment dropout for Hispanic American families, but not for Anglo-American families (Flicker et al.
2008).
Present Study
To date, no meta-analytic review of alliance and outcome in family-involved treatment for youth problems has been published that also focused on moderators of the association between alliance and outcome and included studies published since 2008. The present study meta-analytically summarizes research findings on alliance and treatment outcome in family-involved treatment for youth problems over the past three decades. The purpose is to provide accurate estimates of the associations between the level of alliance and treatment outcome, alliance change scores and treatment outcome, and split alliances and treatment outcome, paying particular attention to both within- and between-study variabilities by performing moderator analyses in a multilevel meta-analysis. The analyses therefore ensure maximum use of the available data and provide valuable insight into the process of building, maintaining and measuring alliance in order to enhance positive outcome in family-involved treatment for youth problems.
Discussion
The Association Between Alliance and Treatment Outcome
Our findings revealed a significant small to medium correlation between the level of alliance and treatment outcome (
r = .18), indicating that higher levels of alliance between the therapist and the family lead to more favorable treatment outcomes. This finding is in line with previous meta-analyses on alliance and treatment outcome in youth psychotherapy, showing comparable overall effect sizes, ranging from
r = .14 to
r = .22 (Karver et al.
2006; McLeod
2011; Shirk and Karver
2003; Shirk et al.
2011). Meta-analyses on alliance in adult psychotherapy have consistently shown somewhat larger overall effect sizes, ranging from
r = .21 to
r = .28 (Horvath and Bedi
2002; Horvath et al.
2011; Martin et al.
2000). Friedlander et al. (
2011) performed a meta-analysis on alliance in couple and family therapy, and found an overall effect size of
r = .26 for both couple and family therapy, and an overall effect size of
r = .24 for family therapy only.
The fact that the present meta-analysis yielded a somewhat smaller overall effect size for family-involved treatment than the meta-analysis by Friedlander et al. (
2011) can be explained by several factors. First, we used stricter inclusion criteria for the family aspect of treatment and included unpublished studies as well as published studies. Second, we used a multilevel model instead of a traditional single-level model as used by Friedlander et al. (
2011). It can therefore be expected that the present study provides a more accurate estimate of the overall effect size.
Furthermore, the previous meta-analysis by Friedlander et al. (
2011) did not report an analysis of publication bias, which may have led to an overestimation of the true effect size. In the present study, correlations between alliance and treatment outcome reported in studies as non-significant without sufficient data to calculate the true effect size were included, with a conservative estimation of zero. As Rosenthal (
1995) pointed out, this conservative estimate of the effect size might lead to an underestimation of the true effect, but simply not using these effect sizes might lead to overestimation of the true effect. To test the hypothesis of underestimation in the present study, we again calculated the overall effect size for the association between the level of alliance and treatment outcome with exclusion of all effect sizes estimated to be zero. The result was a higher overall effect size of
r = .22 (
p < .001). However, the Egger and trim-and-fill analyses indicated that the original overall effect size we found for the association between level of alliance and outcome may still be an overestimation of the true effect size due to publication bias.
Contrary to our expectations, we found only a small correlation between split or unbalanced alliances and outcome, which failed to reach significance. This could indicate that for positive treatment outcome it is irrelevant whether the therapist develops balanced therapeutic relationships with all family members or develops a stronger therapeutic relationship with one of the family members compared to other family members. However, when interpreting the results of the meta-analysis on split alliance and treatment outcome, it should be noted that research on split alliances often lacks a clear definition of the central concept as well as a valid and reliable methodology to the concept. Often, raw difference scores are used to investigate the role of split alliances in treatment outcome. Previous research, however, has shown that these difference scores cannot provide valid and reliable tests of informant discrepancy as a predictor (Bartle-Haring et al.
2012; Laird and De Los Reyes
2013).
Results of the analysis on the association between alliance change scores and treatment outcome showed a trend toward significance indicating a moderate association of r = .281, which is considerably larger than the correlation between level of alliance with fixed moment measures and treatment outcome (r = .18). This might indicate that for the therapist in order to enhance positive treatment outcome, improving alliances with family members during the treatment process might even be more important than developing alliances that remain stable throughout treatment. However, research on alliance change scores related to treatment outcome in family-involved treatment is scarce, and only three studies reporting on alliance change scores could be included in the meta-analysis. This is surprising, given that previous research on alliance in several contexts has shown that alliance can develop in different trajectories during treatment, such as a linear increase in alliance, a fading linear increase in alliance or sudden nonlinear decreases (ruptures) or increases (gains) in alliance (Lange et al. in prep.). How these different developmental trajectories of alliance relate to treatment outcome remains unclear.
Moderating Variables
The results of our study reveal that the association between alliance and outcome was moderated by several characteristics of alliance measures, treatment and study sample. With regard to alliance measures, we found that the correlation between the level of alliance and outcome was stronger when alliance measures at several time points were averaged or added compared to only early, mid- or late treatment measures of alliance. This finding is in line with a study on the alliance–outcome association in psychotherapy for depressed adults, where the average of alliance score measured at session 3–9 explained 14.7% of the outcome variance, whereas single alliance measures of session 3 explained only 4.7% of the outcome variance (Crits-Christoph et al.
2012). Meta-analytic reviews on the alliance–outcome association in adult and youth psychotherapy have consistently reported stronger correlations between alliance measured during late treatment and alliance measured during early or midtreatment (Horvath et al.
2011; McLeod
2011; Shirk and Karver
2003). However, none of these studies reported on multiple alliance measures averaged or added as a category for timing of alliance measure. If we take into account that our meta-analysis on alliance change scores and treatment outcome showed a marginally significant larger effect size compared to the association between level of alliance and outcome, our findings underline the importance of viewing the alliance as a dynamic process rather than a static, single-moment entity.
No moderating effects were found in any of our analyses for type of alliance informant (youth, parent or observer) or alliance construct. These findings are in line with the findings from the meta-analytic review of Shirk et al. (
2011) that did not show moderating effects for any characteristics of the alliance measure. Also, Horvath et al. (
2011) found no moderating effect for alliance rater in the association between alliance and outcome in individual adult psychotherapy. However, other meta-analytic reviews have reported larger effect sizes for therapist rated alliance compared to other sources of alliance measurement (Shirk and Karver
2003), or for parent rated alliance compared to other sources of alliance (McLeod
2011). This might indicate that no consistent conclusion can yet be drawn about the role of alliance source in the alliance–outcome association.
Contrary to our expectations, in the association between level of alliance and outcome we found no moderating effect for type of alliance informant (youth, parent or observer), type of alliance (youth–therapist, parent–therapist or family alliance), construct of alliance (bond, goal, task, within-family) or alliance measures designed specifically for family therapy in order to capture systemic aspects of the alliance. Previous studies that found a smaller or less significant effect on treatment outcome for alliance in family-involved treatment compared to individual treatment have underlined the importance of studying alliance in family therapy with instruments that capture systemic aspects of alliance typical of working with multiple family members (Lange et al. in prep.; McLeod
2011). The rationale behind this point of view is that alliance instruments designed for family therapy might lead to a better understanding of the alliance–outcome association in family-involved treatment. However, research on alliance and outcome using specific family therapy alliance measures is still scarce, and in the present study, only four independent samples using a specific family therapy measure of the alliance could be included in the meta-analysis on level of alliance and outcome. Furthermore, out of these four study samples, three samples contained only a small number of families (
n < 50).
Most of the significant moderating variables were sample characteristics, with different moderators for the three separate meta-analyses. The association between level of alliance and treatment outcome was significantly moderated by average age of youth in the sample, demonstrating stronger correlations when youths were younger. This is in line with findings of McLeod (
2011) and Shirk and Karver (
2003) showing that in youth psychotherapy associations between alliance and outcome were stronger for younger children compared to adolescents. However, it should be noted that in the present study variance in average age of youth in study samples was small, with the lowest average age of 10.6 and the highest average age of 16.1. Most study samples comprised only families with adolescents, some samples comprised adolescents as well as younger children, and no studies were included with families with children in primary school age only. It is unclear whether our study findings can be generalized to families receiving therapy or treatment due to concerns regarding much younger children. In families with younger children, the role of the child in therapy might not be as active as compared to older youth, resulting perhaps in lower correlations between youth alliance and outcome and higher correlations between parent alliance and outcome.
Another moderating sample characteristic in the association between level of alliance and treatment outcome was referral source, showing stronger correlations between alliance and outcome for clients who were help-seeking or recruited for the study compared to samples with mandated clients or a combination of mandated and help-seeking clients. This finding was replicated in the meta-analysis on alliance change scores and treatment outcome. Two recent studies compared alliance processes in family therapy between voluntary and involuntary clients. These studies revealed that initial between-group differences in the emotional bond with the therapist and the within-family alliance did disappear after four sessions of therapy (Sotero et al. in press; Sotero et al.
2016). Between-group differences in agreement on therapeutic goals and tasks, however, remained after the fourth session. Thus, the difference in the alliance–outcome association between self-referred and involuntary clients might be explained by both timing and dimension of alliance measure. However, no research has yet been published on the relation between specific aspects of alliance processes with mandated clients in relation to treatment outcome. Furthermore, in the present meta-analysis only one study could be included with mandated clients only. Five other included studies reported on samples of both mandated and help-seeking clients, with no reports of specific effect sizes for both groups.
It was surprising that no moderating effects were found for problem type or treatment model in the association between level of alliance and outcome. Several meta-analyses have demonstrated moderating effects for problem type (McLeod
2011; Shirk and Karver
2003; Shirk et al.
2011). In the present study, the sample of included studies was very heterogeneous with regard to problem type and treatment model. As a result, several categories for these variables were represented by only one or two studies. Thus, the fact that the moderating effect of problem type and treatment model failed to reach significance might partly be explained by a lack of statistical power. Nevertheless, this finding underlines the importance of training and supervision for therapists in alliance building techniques in addition to training and supervision of specific treatment model techniques.
In contrast to our expectations, both gender and ethnicity of clients did not moderate the association between level of alliance and outcome. With regard to gender, two previous studies on alliance and outcome in family therapy and couple therapy demonstrated that for male adults other aspects of alliance are important in relation to treatment outcome compared to females (Johnson et al.
2002) and that the correlation between alliance and outcome might be stronger when males have a higher level of alliance with their therapist than their female partners (Symonds and Horvath
2004). With regard to ethnicity, one included study on split alliance and outcome shows a stronger correlation between alliance and outcome for Hispanic families compared to Anglo-American families (Flicker et al.
2008). It should be noted, however, that most of the studies included in the present study made no distinction between alliance–outcome correlations for boys and girls, or father and mothers, or between different ethnical groups. Thus, there was no variance between effect sizes within studies with regard to gender or ethnicity. We did, however, find a significant moderating effect for percentage of male adults in the association between alliance change scores and treatment outcome, demonstrating stronger correlations in samples with less male adults. This might indicate that for fathers, the process of alliance improvement is more predictive of treatment outcome than for mothers.
For outcome measures characteristics, no moderating effects were found in any of the investigated associations in contrast to findings of previous meta-analyses on alliance and outcome in youth or adult psychotherapy (Horvath et al.
2011; McLeod
2011; Shirk and Karver
2003). This indicates that alliance is a significant small predictor of treatment outcome in family-involved treatment, regardless of how and when outcome is measured.
Lastly, there was no moderating effect for study quality in the associations between level of alliance and outcome, and alliance change scores and outcome, although there was a trend toward significance in the first association indicating stronger correlations in studies of less quality. This moderating effect was significant in the association between split alliances and outcome.
Limitations of the Study
The present study has several limitations. An important methodological limitation is the small number of studies included in the meta-analyses that investigated the association between split alliances and treatment outcome (five studies reporting on 17 effect sizes) and the association between alliance change scores and treatment outcome (three studies reporting on 15 effect sizes). Therefore, conclusions from these analyses should be interpreted with caution and require future re-evaluation when a larger body of evidence has accumulated.
Second, some categorical variables in the moderator analyses contained relatively few studies, which resulted in insufficient statistical power of the analyses. This was the case for all moderator variables in the associations between split alliances and outcome, and alliance change scores and outcome as a result of the small number of studies included in these meta-analyses. For the association between level of alliance and treatment outcome, the problem of insufficient statistical power might especially apply to problem type, treatment setting, treatment model, referral source and several outcome characteristics.
A final limitation is that in the current meta-analysis, alliance–outcome associations were analyzed across a variety of research designs, ranging from uncontrolled pre-post designs to quasi-experimental designs. It could be reasoned that the strength of alliance–outcome associations differs considerably across research designs. Therefore, future research—based on a larger body of evidence than is currently available—may benefit from a fine-grained analysis of the moderating effect of research designs on the alliance–outcome association in general.
Implications for Future Research
The sample of studies included in the present study shows that the association between alliances processes and treatment outcome has received less attention within specific treatment contexts. Treatment contexts that differing from the regular context of family-involved treatment for youth problems (i.e., family therapy in an outpatient clinic with families seeking help for a specific problem of their adolescent) might lead to different behaviors of clients, demanding different alliance building skills from therapists. Research on alliance in specific contexts, such as home-based interventions, interventions for multiproblem families or families receiving mandated treatment, might lead to a better understanding of how alliance processes are related to outcome within these specific contexts.
Future research on alliance in family-involved treatment could also benefit from investigating the more complex systemic and dynamic aspects of alliance typical of working with families. One of these systemic aspects is the occurrence of split alliances. As pointed out before, the scarce research on split alliances that is available often lacks a clear definition of the concept, and applied methodology in most of these studies might not be appropriate for investigating the role of split alliances in treatment effectiveness. For research on split alliances, applying methods other than using discrepancy scores is recommended, such as multilevel modeling (Bartle-Haring et al.
2012) or polynomial regression (Laird and De Los Reyes
2013).
Furthermore, the use of alliance measures designed specifically for the context of family therapy, such as FTAS (Pinsof et al.
2008) or SOFTA (Friedlander et al.
2006), may help to gain a better understanding of systemic dynamics of alliance in family-involved treatment related to outcome. Although it has been reasoned before that clarifying these systemic dimensions of alliance may help to produce a more accurate estimate of the association between alliance processes and treatment outcome (Friedlander et al.
2006; McLeod
2011), the present study shows that research investigating the within-family or family therapist alliance is still scarce.
Our findings furthermore indicate that research on the role of alliance in family-involved interventions in treatment effectiveness could benefit from viewing the alliance as a dynamic process rather than a static measure at a single time point. However, research on the evolvement of alliance during treatment is still scarce, and questions remain in particular about the relation between specific developmental trajectories of alliance, such as alliance ruptures or sudden alliance gains, and treatment outcome.
Lastly, research on family-involved treatment might benefit from investigating the role of other common factors that have been hypothesized to be important in determining treatment outcome, such as client motivation, expectancies about services and family empowerment (Hoagwood
2005; Karver et al.
2006; Sprenkle and Blow
2004).
Conclusions
We investigated the association between alliance and treatment outcome in family-involved treatment for youth problems by analyzing data from 28 studies reporting on 21 independent study samples. Our findings demonstrate that a stronger alliance is a small but significant predictor of better treatment outcomes, underlining the importance for therapists to develop strong alliances with family members during treatment. The association between alliance and treatment outcome was stronger when youth in treatment was in their early adolescence compared to late adolescence, when clients in the study sample were help-seeking or recruited for the study instead of mandated for treatment and when alliance measures of several time points during treatment were averaged or added.
Results of our study furthermore indicate that growth of alliance during the treatment process might be a stronger predictor of treatment outcome than alliance measured at a single time point or an average of alliance measures over time. The occurrence of split alliances did not predict treatment outcome. However, only few studies reported on the association between split alliances and outcome and most studies lack a clear definition and appropriate methodology to measure split alliances.
Our study underlines the importance for therapists to build strong individual alliances with all family members involved in treatment as well as to pay attention to systemic aspects of the alliance, such as the within-family alliance, when delivering family-involved treatment for youth problems. Furthermore, our study implicates that training and supervision of (family) therapists should focus not solely on specific treatment model techniques, but also on alliance building techniques in the context of working with multiple family members. Building these multiple alliances remains important throughout the treatment process, regardless of the treatment model. Therapists might enhance treatment outcome by monitoring individual as well as within-family alliances, in order to intervene when alliances are problematic.
Future research should focus on the association between alliance and outcome in specific treatment contexts of family-involved treatment, such as home-based interventions and therapy with involuntary clients. Furthermore, future research could benefit from investigating complex aspects of alliance within family-involved treatment, such as the role of within-family alliance, the occurrence of split alliances and alliance ruptures, to gain fuller understanding of the dynamic role of alliance in family-involved treatment in order to enhance positive treatment outcome.